Wednesday, May 2, 2012

Neurons and Creativity

  I've been puzzled ever since I posted "Intuition and Expertise" about a month ago. If you read it, you may remember a central postulate of Daniel Kahneman's book Thinking, Fast and Slow: people use two distinct mechanisms in thinking.  These are a fast and intuitive "System 1" and a slow and analytical "System 2."  It's a model that left me with nagging questions, unaddressed by Kahneman.  Are the two systems merely metaphors that don't have discrete neurological counterparts?  Or is each system instantiated at a specific neurological site?

  I was therefore excited to hear an interview on NPR in which Jonah Lehrer discussed his new book Imagine: How Creativity Works.  He talked not only about intuitive and analytical thinking in creativity, but also about particular neural circuits that are their loci.

  Thankfully, Lehrer is a journalist, not a scientist like Kahneman, so his book is short and relatively crisp.  (Good! I thought. I didn't want to plunge into another tome like Kahneman's.)  The book has two parts, "Alone" and "Together," which respectively address individual and communal creativity.  Just the first part is concerned with the details of neural activity, so I will confine myself mostly to it.  But the other part is of sufficient interest to warrant a brief outline.

  Together.  Lehrer asks why such places as fifth century BCE Athens, fifteenth-century Florence, late sixteenth-century London and today's Silicon Valley came to have "clots of excess genius"—rare agglomerations of creative people.  He maintains that they have in common a vital combination of ingredients: a dense population, relative affluence, and civic and social institutions that encourage creativity. Together, these ingredients foster frequent interactions among imaginative individuals, resulting in their interchanging ideas.  Each individual's creativity, the "alone" kind, is stoked by bumping into other creative individuals at random.  The environment in turn attracts still more of the brightest and most original, driving a positive feedback loop.

  For example, late sixteenth- and early seventeenth-century London had one of the densest populations on earth; a relative lack of censorship; an amazing near-50% literacy rate nurtured by the English Reformation's rendering of the Bible into the vernacular; ardent and supportive theater-goers; and a plethora of coffee houses, which acted as milieux for chance meetings.  The result was a "clot of excess genius" containing the likes of Jonson, Marlowe, Shakespeare, Milton, Kyd, Spenser and Donne.  English literature has never again seen an efflorescence like that in such a short time period.

  Many modern companies try to mimic this clotting phenomenon.  At the insistence of Steve Jobs, for instance, all of the most-frequented spaces at Pixar and Apple—cafes and cafeterias, mailboxes, meeting rooms, even bathrooms—were clumped together centrally, so that people would be forced by happenstance to bump into each other, chatter and exchange ideas.  It seems to work, for Pixar and Apple are among the most creative companies ever established.

  Alone.  Still, while people can be prodded into creative acts by their surroundings, the act itself is usually individual.  That's where System 1 and System 2 thinking come into play.  Lehrer provides a fine insight into how these two forms of thinking occur in the brain.  Here are some of the facts he presents, with a few of my own comments interpolated.

  As I argued in my posting on intuition and expertise, System 1 leads to the Aha! moments that we all experience.  Finding the part of the brain where those epiphanies are generated has recently been made possible by using functional MRI (fMRI) measurements, which perceive increases in neural activity as surges in blood flow to those neurons.  Such measurements show that the anterior superior temporal gyrus (aSTG), a small fold of brain tissue above the right ear, becomes especially active a few seconds before an insight.  The aSTG functions subconsciously, apparently by obsessively searching for relationships among the myriad pieces of data that are stored in the brain.   When it finds a significant relationship, it "lights up" in the fMRI scan, like the light bulb above a cartoon character's head indicating an intuitive flash.  We may trigger an aSTG search because we are stumped in the analysis of a particular problem, probably because we are looking at the wrong data, or it may happen autonomously.  Interestingly, the aSTG seems to be most active when we are relaxed, possibly daydreaming.  That may be why Archimedes got his Eureka! moment while in a bath.

  On the other hand, System 2 thinking, the analytical kind, is in our conscious mind all along; we know that it is happening because it is effortful, concentrated work.  That work takes place in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), located behind the forehead.  The PFC has associated with it a short-term working memory that allows it  to focus on the very pieces of data it presumably needs to solve a problem—akin, I think, to a computer's RAM.  By not paying much attention to the vast amounts of other data in the brain (data "on disk" so to speak), the PFC emphasizes the fine-tuning of an emerging solution, but doesn't provoke epiphanies.  We gain incremental understanding from its activities, piece by piece.

  Part of the PFC may actually inhibit our full creativity: the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), which is most closely associated with impulse control.  It constrains our thoughts and actions so that we don't make fools of ourselves or violate social norms—a sort of straitjacket that the mind places on itself.  It can therefore prevent us from thinking "out of the box."  Tellingly, it is one of the last areas of the brain developed by children, which may explain both their relative lack of social inhibitions and their as-yet unbridled creativity.  fMRI studies of jazz musicians while they are improvising show their DLPFC activity to be suppressed, as if they are purposely inhibiting their inhibitions so as to be able to jam; this suppression doesn't occur when they are merely playing a set piece.

  Another connection, or actually lack of one, between creativity and the PFC is quite amazing.  When we are asleep, the PFC shuts down!  Our analytical abilities and our inhibitions—our sanity checkers—are gone.  The brain is then free to run amok, dreaming uninhibited thoughts and making "insane" connections among its memories.  Occasionally connections that do make sense occur in this maelstrom.  Perhaps that is why we sometimes decide to "sleep on it" when we are trying to solve a particularly vexing problem.

  These few facts about the neurology of thinking are of course not the whole story.  No matter. They are the beginning of an answer to the "nagging questions" Kahneman left me with.  My inner scientist is very pleased.  I love it when observational data can be understood in terms of underlying physical phenomena!